|
|||||||
|
|
||||
|
3
Main Ideas |
Orthonormal Color Matching Functions.
|
Vectorial Sensitivity to narrow-band lights. |
Tristimulus vectors,
including amplitude. |
Early
Color-Matching Data of Guild or Wright. |
Thoughts on Matching Data |
The
primary lights are not unique, and the same facts can be presented in
alternate sets of graphs, an awkward situation. However,
|
Same Color Matches, Varying Overlap among Functions: |
(x, y) Diagram |
So, we think in terms of the chromaticity diagram... | but those primary
colors are still needed. |
Which Wavelengths Act Most Strongly in Mixtures? |
MacAdam, and later
Thornton did calculations like this. Narrow-band lights of constant power are mixed with equal energy light. Some wavelengths perturb the white chromaticity more than others. Thornton coined the term "Prime Colors" for the 3 wavelengths that act most strongly. |
Color
Matching
Functions Are in Fact Relatively Stable When the Primary Wavelengths Are Changed. |
|
Later
Thornton found:
Michael
Brill's new theorem:
When one primary
wavelength is changed (say the red
wavelength only) then the associated (red) color matching function
changes only in scale, not in shape.(For a more leisurely discussion of shifting primary wavelengths, please click here.) |
Prime
Colors Must Relate to the Overlap of Red and Green Sensitivities... |
Red and green cone functions are highly
overlapping. |
Subtracting green
from red gives peaks to account for Prime Colors, but the scaling is
arbitrary. |
Achromatic
sensitivity, y-bar, is a linear combination of red and green. Find a
second combination that is orthogonal to it. |
One
Degree of Freedom Remains, to Re-mix ω1 and ω2, But Keep the Mixtures Orthogonal. |
|
We start with ω1 and ω2,
which are
linear combinations of red and green cone functions, and are
orthonormal. Other orthonormal pairs of functions can be generated by a
rotation matrix: |
Now
Make a Parametric Plot of ω2 vs ω1. Bingo, the Shape is Invariant. |
Vectorial
Sensitivity to
Wavelength. |
For each λ, the
eye's sensitivity is a vector, (ω2, ω1) . The spectrum locus is the eye's vectorial sensitivity to color. It is not a boundary. The spectrum locus is alternatively the vectorial color of narrow-band lights, at unit power. |
Prime colors are the wavelengths where radius is a local maximum! |
Stalking
Prime Colors in the 2 Dimensions of Red and Green. |
Vectorial
Sensitivity to
Wavelength, Now in 3 Dimensions. |
Features
of the Orthonormal System |
Applications |
Color
Rendering by Light Sources |
Oh,
Yes, Calculating Tristimulus Vectors. |
Let L be a light, that is a Spectral
Power Distribution.
The calculation
is essentially the same, but the benefits of the orthonormal color
matching functions are tremendous!
|
Issues
Demystified: |
Special
Credit |
William A. Thornton Michael H.
Brill (But
MacAdam gets a demerit for disparaging Cohen's work.) Calculations were
done with O-matrix software. |
William A. Thornton
|
Jozef B. Cohen
|
Background |
General
Background, including Thornton's
and Cohen's work is in Render Asking: James A. Worthey, "Color rendering: asking the question," Color Research and Application 28(6):403-412, December 2003. Mathematical detail is in Render Calc: James A. Worthey, "Color rendering: a calculation that estimates colorimetric shifts," Color Research and Application 29(1):43-56, February 2004. See http://www.jimworthey.com !
|
Seldom
Asked Questions (links) |
Stop |
Scroll
No Farther |
Material Below Addresses Obscure Questions |
2
colors, C1
and C2 are set to equal power. The mixture is m. |
Cone
Sensitivities, Approximately Smith-Pokorny Primaries |
|
||
|
|
||
|
|
||
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
||
|
|